Luiz Fernando Bertolucci, MD, BSc, is a physiatrist and biologist, a co-founder of Núcleo An- thropos of Integrative Medicine (UNIFESP, São Paulo State Federal University), a Certified Advanced Rolfer™, and a member of the Rolf Institute™ anatomy faculty. He is co-creator and teacher of the Myofascial Release training offered through the ABR (Brazilian Rolfing® Association) and the creator of the Muscle Repositioning technique.
This article was previously published in the Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies in July 2011.
Note on Terminology
Pandiculation (from the Latin: pandiculare = stretching) is most often used to denote both the yawning and the involuntary stretching of somatic muscles, which together characterize the stretch- yawning syndrome (SYS). Though the yawn and the stretch are associated phenomena, each can occur independent of the other. Most studies about SYS focus on yawning, although the oft-accompanying stretch is occasionally described. It is less common for studies focus on the stretch component alone. In this text, SYS will be used to denote the combined behavior, while pandiculation will be used to denote the stretch behavior by itself.1 Although such use is not the most common, the author rejected the term stretch to denote the involuntary stretching part of the SYS because it can be confused with voluntary muscle lengthening, which is part of a wide range of sports and bodywork activities, and is an entirely distinguishable phenomenon.
Where other authors are quoted directly, the terminology is their own.
Summary
Pandiculation is the involuntary stretching of the soft tissues, which occurs in most animal species and is associated with transitions between cyclic biological behaviors, especially the sleep-wake rhythm (Walusinski, 2006). Yawning is considered a special case of pandiculation that affects the musculature of the mouth, respiratory system and upper spine (Baenninger, 1997). When, as often happens, yawning occurs simultaneously with pandiculation in other body regions (Bertolini and Gessa, 1981; Lehmann, 1979; Urba-Holmgren et al., 1977) the combined behavior is referred to as the stretch-yawning syndrome (SYS).
SYS has been associated with the arousal function, as it seems to reset the central nervous system to the waking state after a period of sleep and prepare the animal to respond to environmental stimuli (Walusinski, 2006).
This paper explores the hypothesis that the SYS might also have an auto-regulatory role regarding the locomotor system: to maintain the animal’s ability to express coordinated and integrated movement by regularly restoring and resetting the structural and functional equilibrium of the myofascial system.
It is now recognized that the myofascial system is integrative, linking body parts, as the force of a muscle is transmitted via the fascial structures well beyond the tendonous attachments of the muscle itself (Huijing and Jaspers, 2005). It is argued here that pandiculation might preserve the integrative role of the myofascial system by (a) developing and maintaining appropriate physiological fascial interconnections and (b) modulating the pre-stress state of the myofascial system by regularly activating the tonic musculature.
The ideas presented here initially arose from clinical observations during the practice of a manual therapy called Muscular Repositioning (MR) (Bertolucci, 2008; Bertolucci and Kozasa, 2010a; Bertolucci, 2010b). These observations were supplemented by a review of the literature on the subject.
Pandiculation: Determining and Maintaining Neural Circuitry and Peripheral Motor Effectors
Pandiculation is an old and almost ubiquitous behavior that occurs in similar form and circumstances across a wide spectrum of species (Baenninger, 1997). The regularity and vigor of pandiculatory movements suggest that they might be physiologically significant. Walunsinki notes that according to Darwin’s concepts, the cost of a behavior with high metabolic demand is likely to be outweighed by some adaptive benefit (Walusinski, 2006).
Indeed, the phylogeny and ontogeny of pandiculation reveal its likely role in the development and maintenance of motor function, in both its structural and neural aspects. Fraser (Fraser, 1989a), in connection with ultrasound fetal studies on sheep, refers to fetal pandiculation as a mechanism that influences functional determination of the moving parts of the musculoskeletal system and contributes to articular development and maintenance. He also identifies a bodily care and self-maintenance function of pandiculation, which restores muscle homeostasis in poultry, dogs, cats and horses, among other animals (Fraser, 1989a). In ostriches, a similar “maintenance” pandiculation has been described by Sauer and Sauer (Sauer and Sauer, 1967).
In man, pandiculation is also ontogenetically precocious, startingas early as 12weeks of gestation (deVries et al., 1982). It has been associated with the development of motor neural circuitry (Lagercrantz and Ringsted, 2001; Marder and Rehm, 2005; Briscoe and Wilkinson, 2004) and associated musculoskeletal effectors (de Vries et al., 1982; Walusinski et al., 2005). In fact, individual muscles are differentiated from primitive mesodermal condensations only after the establishment of their neural connections (Sadler, 1995), based on their specific motor actions, as discussed below:
Muscle development begins with primitive myoblasts, which aggregate and give rise to the primary myotubes. Around the 10th week of gestation, the primary myotubes are invaded by spinal cord motor neurons, which give the primary myotubes contractile capacity. Within the primary myotubes, additional myoblasts aggregate, eventually giving rise to secondary myotubes, both sharing the same basement membrane at this stage. Later in fetal development, the secondary myotubes connect with tendon structures, at which time they develop their own basement membranes and become independent structures (Jones et al., 2004).
The differentiation of primary and secondary myotubes into discrete motor unit populations is implied by the differentiation of separate encasing fascial layers and gliding interfaces, which enclose and allow independent motion of their contents; i.e., discrete fascial linings—and the areolar matrices between them, which allow them to glide against each other—are established upon functional grounds.
Such mechanical-structural coupling is not confined to soft tissues; it can be extended to bone (Wolff, 1986), joint surface shapes (Kapandji, 1987) and virtually every tissue under mechanical stress (Moore, 2003; Silver et al., 2003). Besides shape, the molecular compositions of musculoskeletal structures are also functionally determined, involving the phenomenon of mechanotransduction (Ingber, 2006, Stevens-Tuttle et al., 2008; Banes et al., 1999), further discussed below.
In summary of the above, repetitive motion gradually determines shapes and compositions of moving structures, as well as their associated neural control pathways. The precociousness and stability of pandiculation suggest its contribution to such development.
Neuropharmacology and Neurophysiology of SYS: An Arousal Function?
The SYS has been associated with the maintenance of arousal and attention; i.e., it sets and maintains the central nervous and locomotor systems so that the animal is able to perceive environmental stimuli and respond to them with appropriate motor actions (Baenninger, 1997; Walusinski, 2006; Askenasy, 1989).
Although no particular cerebral structure has been definitively associated with yawning (Argiolas and Melis, 1998), because yawning is seen in anencephalic newborns with only the medulla oblongata (Heusner, 1946), the brainstem appears to be involved. In addition, pharmacological and clinical evidence suggests the involvement of the hypothalamus, bulbus and pons. Since the 1960s, injection of various substances (e.g., adrenocorticotropin [ACTH], a-melanocyte stimulating hormone [a-MSH] and related peptides) into the CNS, has been shown to stimulate SYS activity in laboratory animals (Argiolas and Melis, 1998). More recently, it has been shown that rats’ stereotypical yawning, including trunk stretching (SYS), can be triggered by stimulation of the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus via electrical or chemical (nitric oxidereleasing compounds) means (Sato-Suzuki et al., 1998).
Today, it is commonly believed that, once stimulated, PVN oxytocinergic neurons release oxytocin into extrahypothalamic structures; and that these structures, in turn, mediate SYS and arousal responses (Walusinski, 2006). As SYS involves various behavioral and autonomic events, it is assumed that the PVN integrates the outputs of various origins and creates efferents on at least five different levels: respiratory, cardiovascular, facial motor, spinal motor and arousal systems (Sato-Suzuki et al., 1998), which are responsible, respectively, for the reactions of deep breath, drop in blood pressure, mouth opening, trunk stretching, and arousal response. Other neurotransmitters, including orexins, acetylcholine, dopamine, serotonin, and opioid peptides have also been shown to mediate the expression of SYS behavior (Argiolas and Melis, 1998; Sato-Suzuki et al., 2002).
Sato-Suzuki’s model on yawning/arousal responses in rats illustrates the main CNS pathways possibly involved in SYS (Sato-Suzuki et al., 2002) (Fig. 1).
Figure 1: Schematic representation of Sato-Suzuki’s model on yawning/arousal responses mediated through the PVN in rats. orexin neurons from the lateral hypothalamic area (LHA) may directly activate neurons in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN), which in turn project to the locus coeruleus (LC) or the basal forebrain. Oxytocinergic neurons from the PVN project to the lower brainstem, including the facial nucleus and respiratory related neurons which are implicated in the yawning response. oXT, oxytocin; oX, orexin; NA, noradrenaline; Ach, acetylcholine. Reprinted with permission from: Ikuko Sato-Suzuki, Ichiro Kita, yoshinari Seki, mitsugu oguri, Hideho Arita. Behavioural Brain Research 128 (2002) 169e177 Cortical arousal induced by microinjection of orexins into the paraventricular nucleus of the rat.
Pandiculation can be considered a Modal Action Pattern (MAP) (Rosenbloom, 1994), an orderly sequence of reflexive behaviors, within which category evolutionarily important instinctual activities such as feeding, mating and other social behaviors important to survival are classified. As such, pandiculation seems to be elicited by complex array or sequence of stimuli, which might include both exteroceptive signals (e.g., light-darkness) and interoceptive ones (e.g., circadian endocrine cycles and somatic interoception) (Walusinski, 2006).
SYS Phenotype
The SYS has a similar and stereotyped phenotype along the evolutionary scale, having remained virtually unchanged. Yawning (Fig. 2) starts with a long and deep inhale, reaches a peak, and concludes with a short exhale. Respiratory, mouth, neck and upper spine muscles engage in co-contraction, simultaneously stiffening the joints and stretching the myofascial tissues (Walusinski, 2006).
The few references to pandiculation in the existing literature describe it as a series of coordinated actions that unfold sequentially, building up soft tissue contractile tension to a peak, at which point the joints of the limbs and trunk are maximally extended—or, alternatively, the trunk is arched in flexion (Fraser, 1989b). After the peak, the soft tissue tension level plummets, yielding a sense of pleasure and well-being (See section on Pleasure, below). The actions can be regional or involve the whole body, and are often bilaterally symmetrical (Fraser, 1989b).
SYS: Compensatory Response to Temporary Stiffness or Immobility?
The patterns of full body pandiculation are, in general, similar to the ones used in striding and righting behaviors (Fraser, 1989b)—i.e., they emulate ordinary functional movements—while pandiculation of limited bodily regions seem to be a corrective response to the stiffness induced by temporary positional stress or immobility.
During turnover metabolism extracellular matrix (ECM) components are continuously both reabsorbed and synthesized. Molecular interactions between glicoseaminoglicans (GAGs), fibers and other components continuously change structural mechanical properties. The weaving of protocollagen monomers into fibers is spontaneous, driven by hydrophilic interactions of hydrogen bonding between water and amino acid side chains. All that is necessary for this process are the physiologic conditions of temperature, pH, ionic strength and degree of hydration (Leikin et al., 1995; Giraud-Guille et al., 2003).
Mechanical stresses are the main determinants of form and relationship in this continuous and spontaneous reweaving; i.e., movement is crucial to the maintenance of the appropriate form and function of the ECM (Kjær et al., 2005, 2006, 2009; Tomiosso et al., 2005). For example, dense tissue is deposited in response to the mechanical need for tensional resistance, while areolar tissue is renewed where gliding is required. Without mechanical input, the ECM would be laid down amorphously and its configuration would not meet physiological requirements.
For the configuration of the ECM to be physiologically appropriate, the mechanical input that stimulates it must first be appropriate. For example, a long striding movement will remain possible only to the extent it is sufficiently expressed, because only such expression will stimulate a supportive ECM configuration (Kjær, 2004; Kjær et al., 2009, Heinemeier et al., 2007).
But, most of the time, animals are not expressing their optimal qualities of movement, such as running at maximum speeds and attaining maximum range of movement (ROM) in the joints. What’s more, sleep imposes a regular period of immobilization, to which the patterns of collagen weaving and ECM relationships will accommodate. This suggests a continual tendency to “tie up” the animal’s entire structure: cross-linking of collagen fibers directed by sub-optimal movements can limit function by diminishing (a) tissue length and strength and (b) the internal gliding capacity between parts necessary for attaining optimal motor potential. Unless it is counteracted, this process could lead to progressive loss of ROM in the joints and stiffening of tissues, both of which might compromise tissue health by restricting fluid distribution and distorting cell shape – and thus inhibiting appropriate gene expression. The mechanically driven modulation of genetic activity will be further discussed in the section on Mechanotransduction, below.
Figure 2: yawning is commonly accompanied by pandiculation of the upper extremities.
Pandiculation, with its specific and vigorous muscle activity, might be a means to compensate for the mechanical signals delivered by rest periods and sub-optimal movements. Fraser mentions, in connection with his studies of pandiculation among various species, that it might be considered a feedback from stiffness, and possibly be triggered by extended periods of immobility in asymmetrical positions. He concludes that if the body tends to stiffen, pandiculation “can serve to restore the limb (and related musculature) to an original (homeostatic) state (Fraser, 1989a, 1989b)”. In fact, SYS has much in common with other homeostatic functions, as discussed immediately below.
Emotional Motor Systems and SYS
In humans, voluntary motricity involves the concerted activity of all constituents of the motor system. Movement planning is translated into actual motor responses through a chain of neural activity that starts at the prefrontal cortex, and then reaches the premotor area and the primary motor cortex sequentially (Kandel et al., 2000; Hallett, 2007).
Neurons in the primary motor cortex project their axons directly to motor neurons via the cortico-spinal tract. As these axons descend, they form the medullary pyramids; thus, the entire projection is also called the pyramidal tract. The motor information the pyramidal tract carries is modulated not only by sensory information, but also by information from lower motor regions (basal ganglia, thalamus, midbrain, cerebellum, spinal cord), which information allows voluntary movements to be smooth, precise and well coordinated (Kandel et al., 2000). The modulation of information in the pyramidal tract is mainly unconscious and automatic; i.e., the subcortical centers provide the unconscious, involuntary and patterned components essential to movement control (Jacobs and Horak, 2007; Guyton and Hall, 2006; Takakusaki et al., 2003), in contrast to the volitional components of motor action governed by the cortical centers.
In general, the lower the structure, the simpler the motor information it conveys. For example, the spinal cord is at the lowest level; and the information it conveys serves tomediate simple reflexes related to rhythmic automatisms such as locomotion and scratching. Higher centers mediate more complex motor activity in a hierarchical organization of complexity (Kandel et al., 2000). For example, while lesions of the primary motor cortex can produce contralateral hemiplegia in humans, some postural and stereotyped involuntary movements may be preserved (Kandel et al., 2000).
Similarly, components of certain instinctual behaviors can be executed without the intervention or modulation by the cortical centers. Instinctual behaviors evolved to guarantee the continuity of organisms’ lineages, maintain internal homeostasis, and insure successful breeding (Dentona et al., 2009).
They are mediated primarily by the limbic system and include those life-supporting activities (e.g., feeding, self-defense, sex) collectively named emotional behaviors (Guyton and Hall, 2006; Kandel et al., 2000).
In experimental animal models, stimulation of limbic structures, notably the hypothalamus and the amygdala, has elicited various emotional behaviors (Kandel et al., 2000,Guyton and Hall, 2006). – even in the absence of the cerebral cortex. For example, after decortication, an investigative procedure in which the connections between cortical and subcortical structures are severed, the latter continue to function without inhibitory influence of the former. Decorticated animals can feed themselves, express rage and fight, and have sexual intercourse (Magoun and Ranson, 1938; Smith, 1939; Guyton and Hall, 2006), which shows that the limbic system can produce such behaviors in the absence of cortical participation.
In fact, the limbic system sends diffuse and innumerable projections to the medulla. It is a system unto itself, able to produce motor activity independent of the voluntarily driven pyramidal system. Holtstege describes it as the emotional motor system because limbically regulated behaviors depend on the emotional state of the animal. The functions governed by the limbic system include different types of involuntary movements associated with olfaction and eating, such as licking, chewing, and swallowing; clonic and rhythmical movements (e.g., locomotion, shivering); sexual function; vocalization, laughing, and crying; and defense reflexes, among others (Guyton and Hall, 2006; Holstege, 1992). Indeed, in laboratory animals various stereotyped behaviors have been reproduced with electrical or chemical stimulation of mesencephalic nuclei, particularly the PVN (paraventricular nucleus) of the hypothalamus. Chewing, licking, lordosis in females, penile erection, and grooming have been observed (Argiolas et al., 2000; De Wied, 1999; Vergoni et al., 1998). The SYS is among these behaviors produced by the limbic system, and its phylogenic longevity suggests an adaptive function.
One function of the PVN is to integrate the central and peripheral autonomic systems. The PVN projects onto all autonomic neurons in the brainstem and spinal cord, given that it is involved in various autonomic and endocrine functions relative to the maintenance of homeostasis (Kandel et al., 2000).
These functions include: production of cortisol via secretion of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF); production of oxytocin and vasopressin (hormones involved in milk production and water balance respectively); and regulation of factors such as food intake, heart rate, and blood pressure (Kandel et al., 2000; Holstege, 1992). That the SYS is mediated by the PVN suggests that the SYS might serve some general homeostatic function, which functions are carried out autonomously by the emotional motor system.
In man, the workings of the emotional motor system are illustrated by the involuntary movements of patients with voluntary motor pathway lesions.
For instance, when hemiplegic patients yawn, they have been observed to raise involuntarily an otherwise plegic arm (Graham, 1982; Töpper et al., 2003; Stewart, 1921), with the muscles of the paralyzed limb activated nearly as much as those of a normal limb would be in a voluntary movement (Omam et al., 1989). Also observed in hemiplegic patients are sinkinesias, in which voluntary movement of one part produces involuntary movement of another; e.g., thigh flexion and arm abduction during sneezing, or arm movements during micturation (Walshe, 1923). Similarly, patients with voluntary facial palsy show facial movements while laughing (Hopf et al., 1992; Töpper et al., 1995; Chernev et al., 2009), and patients incapable of voluntarily opening their mouths do indeed open them while yawning (Askenasy, 1989).
Taken together, the experimental and clinical observations summarized above illustrate that automatic (emotional) motor behaviors can express their patterns independent of the voluntary somato- motor system. Because in normal subjects the former is under the inhibitory influence of the latter (Marder and Rehm, 2005; Pincus and Tucker, 1974), involuntary limbic behaviors tend to appear when volitionalmotor circuits are damagednaturally (as in hemiplegia) or artificially (as in decortication). The fact that normal human motor behavior includes the modulation of instinctual drives (Smith, 1992) might explain why SYS behavior tends to decrease as the person ages.
Pandiculation Versus Ordinary Stretching: Automatic Versus Volitional Motor Actions
If we attend to our interoceptive sensations, our experience tells us that pandiculation and SYS exhibit peculiar motor recruitment. If one “yawns” on purpose, one’s internal sensations are quite different from those elicited by a spontaneous yawn. Similarly, the sensations produced by spontaneous pandiculation are different from those that accompany either “volitional pandiculation” or volitional soft tissue stretching.
The patterns of volitional stretching are cognitively established and the action purposely performed. They often involve relaxation of the muscles through a diminution of their actions: the subject muscle is elongated passively, as a result of either gravity or the activity of opposing muscles.
By contrast, the patterns of pandiculation are automatic. Through intense and involuntary deep muscle cocontractions, the soft tissues actively elongate themselves against the bony structures as the joints are stiffened. Each movement within the pattern emerges in sequence, apparently from the recruitment of a mosaic of reflexes, the sequence of which can neither be anticipated nor purposely performed. Just as a spontaneous yawn feels quite different from a deliberate imitation of one, spontaneous pandiculation feels quite different from a voluntary pandiculation- like stretch. Because the voluntary and emotional motor systems have discrete neural pathways, pandiculation’s distinctive internal sensations might be attributable to the motor unit recruitment sequences dedicated to automatic movement patterns. Indeed, the contrast between interoceptive experiences during automatic versus volitional motor actions has been documented (Hommel, 2009). What’s more, operation of the hierarchically higher volitional system can inhibit that of the lower automatic system; this inhibition can disrupt the characteristic spontaneity of the SYS in favor of a cognitively directed stretch.
Automatic arm abduction during yawning in hemiplegia shows the non-volitional nature of SYS: the patients did not show any arm movement when they imitated a yawn (Töpper et al., 2003). In other words, voluntarily imitation of the automatic motor pattern (via the cortico-spinal system) will not reproduce immediately the instinctive patterns originated in the limbic system (via the emotional motor system). Moreover, if the motor patterns are discrete, their physiological effects should be discrete, as well.
The importance of stretching to the maintenance of musculoskeletal health is well-known. In humans, each of the myriad of physical fitness regimens that include stretching has its own rationale; and although all muscle groups should be stretched, different regimens address particular problems and are intended to compensate for various patterns of muscle shortness or consequent joint mobility restriction. But how do animals in the wild maintain musculoskeletal health? They perform no voluntary stretching and still maintain their motor capabilities. Might SYS be responsible?
If so, and if it were possible to stimulate SYS, might SYS be employed to achieve therapeutic goals? Various somatic practices encourage the SYS because of its apparent homeostatic effects, e.g., Hanna Somatics, Joyflexing, Eutonia (Hanna, 2004; Johnson, 2002; Vishnivetz, 1995). In Eutonia, the SYS is observed to be evoked by certain attentional states and forms of mechanical stimulation. Similarly, the specific mechanical stimulation of Muscle Repositioning might also stimulate the SYS. (see section on responses induced by MR).
Pleasure and Health
Ancient biological behaviors associated with the maintenance of homeostasis are directed through interoception—the sensory experience reflective of the physiological condition (Craig, 2003). Sensory experiences of displeasure and pleasure define the affective qualities of stimuli, which influence an animal’s behavior (Guyton and Hall, 2006; Bozarth, 1994). The positive affects of pleasurable experiences support many life-supporting behaviors: satisfaction of hunger and thirst, sexual intercourse, and vesicle and bowel evacuation are examples of instinctive behaviors that, once accomplished, reward the animal with an experience of pleasure, which biologically reinforces their expression. Preservation of health (salutogenesis) is intimately linked to the perception of positive affects (Esch and Stefano, 2004). Such positive affect states are closely related to ancient subneocortical limbic brain regions common among humans and other mammals, in which various neuropeptides appear to mediate homeostatic satisfactions (Burgdorf and Panksepp, 2006; Vincent, 1994; Cabanac, 1992).
The instinctive behaviors, contributing as they do to the maintenance of the internal milieu, can be considered homeostatic drives (Sherwood, 2010), a category within which pandiculation may also be included. Not only has pandiculation been associated with pleasure and well-being (Fraser, 1989a; Sauer and Sauer, 1967; Russel and Fernandez-Doz, 1997; Walusinski, 2006; Steward, 1921), but it also shares with the other homeostatic drives involvement of the PVN nucleus of the hypothalamus. Homeostasis is maintained chiefly by the parasympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system (Recordati and Bellini, 2004), and increased parasympathetic activity has been detected during SYS (Askenasy and Askenasy, 1996).
What’s more, the frequency of SYS is correlated with degrees of health or convalescence: Fraser (Fraser, 1989b) notes that pandiculation is absent in animals with some systemic illnesses, but returns as the animal recovers. Similarly, in recovering hemiplegic patients, SYS and synkinesias characteristically re-emerge (Töpper et al., 2003; Hwang et al., 2005) in advance of voluntary limb movements. Notably, patterns of involuntary synkinesias have been proposed as a parameter for assessment of poststroke hemiparesis recovery, as the timing of how the patterns emerge seems to be related to the patient’s functional outcomes (Hwang et al., 2005). During coma, yawning foretells the approach of wakefulness (Braunwald et al., 1987). Therapeutic application of transcranial electromagnetic fields in the treatment of Parkinson’s disease and multiple sclerosis has elicited SYS. In a multiple sclerosis investigation, SYS reappeared in those patients whose disease had gone into remission in response to treatment (Sandyk 1999, Sandyk, 1998).
However, excessive pandiculation is associated with certain diseases and the use of certain drugs (Askenasy, 1989). This suggests a possible distinction between a complete (and successful) and an incomplete (and unsuccessful) pandiculation. Perhaps the former, having fulfilled its purpose, physiologically recurs; while the latter, seeking completion, pathologically repeats. Consider, for example, the palpable frustration that accompanies the interruption of a yawn or sneeze!
Mechanotransduction, Tensegrity and Health
Arguably, a cell’s function is fundamentally dependent on its shape. No longer considered mere gelatinous cytoplasm enveloped by a flexible membrane, the cell is now believed to be structured by an internal cytoskeleton consisting of a complex array of microtubules and microfilaments (Ingber, 2008a). Mechanical stimuli govern the synthesis of the cytoskeleton, which determines the cell’s form (Patwari and Lee, 2008; Bischofs et al., 2008). What’s more, mechanical links between the cytoskeleton, on the one hand, and cellular components such as protein synthesis machinery (e.g., ribosomes and mRNA) and membrane receptors, on the other hand, mechanically modulate the components’ functions by a mechanism known as mechanotransduction (Ingber, 2008b; Bischofs et al., 2008; Chiquet et al., 2009).
Through mechanotransduction, mechanical stimuli are transduced into chemical responses (Humphrey and Delange, 2004), such as protein synthesis. By modulating gene expression, the activity of membrane surface receptors and ion channels, and related cellular functions, mechanical forces govern the cell’s development, maintenance, functions and fate (e.g., Dahl et al., 2010; Chiquet et al., 2009; Patwari and Lee, 2008; Haudenschild et al., 2009).
A cell’s shape, molecular constitution and functions reflect the mechanical stresses put upon it. For example, in response to particular dynamic mechanical load patterns, human mesenchymal stem cells enable distinctive gene expression patterns, which in turn produce changes in cell shape and volume (Haudenschild et al., 2009). It appears that each tissue is developed and maintained to bear the stresses to which it is exposed; e.g., tension stimulates both fibroblastic and osteogenic genes, while compression stimulates genes associated with chondrogenesis (Haudenschild et al., 2009). Evidence suggests that mechanical stimuli alone can even help a genomic- defective cell to attain a normal phenotype (Gieni et Hendzel, 2008).
But because cells are not isolated, cell shape is also influenced by the cell’s relationship to the extracellular matrix (ECM) surrounding it. Through interactions at adhesive sites on the cell membrane, components of the ECM affect the net mechanical outcome of the externally acting forces (Streuli, 2009; Bischofs et al., 2008). For example, fibroblasts and other adherent cells sense the physical parameters (e.g., rigidity) of the ECM and modify their protein expression accordingly, and thus alter their own morphology (Do¨bereiner et al., 2005; Chiquet et al., 2009).
In connection with the influence of the ECM, note that it seems to behave as a liquid crystal (Giraud- Guille et al., 2003; Giraud-Guille, 1992; Bouligand, 1972; Kreis and Boesch, 1994). Therefore, the growth, shape and function of living tissue—not unlike those of macromolecular crystals produced in reactorsalso seem to be governed by mechanical stimuli independent of biological activity (Lappa, 2003). Thus, mechanical stresses seem to direct the ECM’s composition and form (and therefore the organism’s shape and patterns of mobility) both biologically (via mechanotransduction) and non-biologically (via its liquidcrystalline properties).
Macroscopically, the ECM can be conceptualized as a fibrous skeleton (Bienfait, 1987) linked to the bony skeleton. These two skeletons are believed to constitute a tensegrity structure (Ingber, 2008a, 1998): the bones, which resist compression, maintain the distance between joints and counteract the basal tension (pre-stress) imposed by the soft tissues. The soft tissues are the tensional elements, which maintain the relative positions of the bones. Tensegrity exists on smaller scales, as well; i.e., structural hierarchies link the macroscopic assemblage of bones and soft tissue to the mechanosensitive cytoskeleton, which is linked to the cellular components governing gene expression (Ingber, 2006). Movement and posture—acting simultaneously in macroscale and microscale— mechanoregulate cell function and ECM form and relations (Kjær, 2004; Kjær et al., 2009, Heinemeier et al., 2007).
The variety of locomotion on display in any crowded street illustrates how individual human movement is. Within the wide variety of motor patterns, one observes qualities of movement: coordinated, fluid, and aligned movement, on the one hand—and encumbered, abrupt and misaligned movement, on the other hand. Not all qualities of movement present equally beneficial stimuli for cell shape and function, and positive and negative stresses might play their parts in health and disease.
Perhaps optimal movement is that which properly distributes mechanical stress through the various tissues and cells, stimulating them according to their needs. Each cell requires particular mechanical stimuli to its normal function (Ingber, 2006), and the structural and mechanical characteristics of the ECM are the ever- changing products of the various mechanical stresses imposed upon it.
In short, the mechanical balance between hard and soft tissues dictates stress distribution, which plays a key role in cell shape and metabolism. In pandiculation, the intense mechanical stimuli produced by forceful co-contraction of antagonist muscle groups might serve as appropriate organizing signals to the cells and tissues by re-optimizing the mechanical conditions of their environment.
Optimal Mechanical Stimuli Might Maintain Potential for Optimal Motor Function
The vigorous mechanical stimulus of SYS produces maximum soft tissue span and joint extension, which, in turn, maximize body dimensions; e.g., yawning enhances pharyngeal diameter fourfold and separates vocal cords to a degree that “cannot be noticed in any other moment of life (Walusinski, 2006)”. SYS might be a biological compensation for periods of immobility and/or vicious body positions, restoring the animal’s mobility by breaking up abnormal ECM cross-links formed by inactivity or suboptimal activity.
Abnormal ECM cross-links might shorten soft tissues and reduce internal lubrication, leading to motor rigidity. SYS might restore soft tissue length and normal internal gliding movements, at the same time as mechanotransduction maintains tissue composition by matching ECM constituents to the mechanical demands of the cells, thus protecting the cells from mechanical damage (Banes et al., 1999; Ingber, 2006).
SYS and Arousal: Restoration Of Postural Tonus And Tensegrity
Yawning is reportedly more common in carnivores, which exhibit pronounced cycles of rest and activity, than in herbivores, the behavior of which exhibits less frequent cycles of arousal (Baenninger, 1997). This is further evidence of the arousal function of SYS. Upon awakening, the reticular formation relays activating stimuli not only to the thalamo-cortical systems, but also to the postural and locomotor systems. This activation makes the fight-flight response readily available (Walusinski, 2006).
The fact that animals most often awaken immediately following REM sleep (which is characterized by muscle atonia), led Walunsinky (Walusinski, 2006) to postulate an opposing relationship between REM sleep and SYS, during which strong muscular contractions elicit an immediate shift of the previous neural patterns controlling the REM sleep, facilitating the appearance of a functional network appropriate for the control of the wakeful motor pattern. Apparently, SYS restores to the myofascial system the elevated level of tonus required for activity in gravity: because in sleep the myofascia is slack and relaxed, the body segments must be reassembled upon awakening before the organism can move properly in the field of gravity.
In a tensegrity structure, the orderly distribution of prestress permits rapid mechanical communication throughout the entire body. Whatever the entry point and direction of an external mechanical stimulus, the reciprocal tensions within the body allow the stimulus to reach and affect mechanoreceptors throughout the entire system. A tensegrity structure thus exhibits much greater mechanosensitivity than a slack system, in which the influence of external stimuli remains local. The absence of slack in a well-balanced tensegrity structure also means that mechanical communication is virtually instantaneous. This facilitates fast and accurate motor responses (Ingber, 2006, Ingber, 2008b; Chen and Ingber, 1999), the adaptive value of which is evident. Such structural coupling among elements in a tensegrity structure, named “morphological communication”, has already been proposed even to assist in controlling movement, alongside the nervous system (Rieffel et al., 2010).
For the basal tension of a tensegrity structure to be efficiently distributed, the links among the tension elements must be precisely located. In addition, the compression elements must approximate precisely relative to each other, which requires both free articular ROM and free gliding among associated—and precisely located—myofascial structures. A functional combination of linking among tensional elements with gliding among compressional elements is essential to the “assembly” of bony and fibrous skeletons that exhibit full tensegrity properties. Each body form (particular arrangement of intersegmental relative positions) implies a certain combination of precisely located structural linking and gliding configurations to distribute pre-stress evenly. The immense variety of body forms that movement implies, each one affected by gravity, allows us to imagine how innumerable are the linking-gliding configurations.
Perhaps the vigorous co-contractions of pandiculation systematically reshape the structural linkage among segments and simultaneously signal the cells (via mechanotransduction) to synthesize the ECM components required to maintain the appropriate environment (Fig. 3). If so, pandiculation might help restore optimal musculoskeletal arrangements, and thus optimize motor capabilities.
The Pandiculation Connection: Yoga and Martial Arts
The downward dog position (Fig. 4), like many yoga asanas, is reminiscent of an animal pandiculation position (Iyengar, 1979). In fact, some say yoga is derived from automatic and spontaneous actions of sages deep in meditation, and that yoga should be practiced spontaneously (Muni, 1994). Eastern martial arts might also have a connection with pandiculation. Qi Gong, for instance, requires the body to be fortified with automatic (involuntary) tonus in the deep postural muscles at the same time the superficial muscles associated with voluntary activity are relaxed (Fig. 5).
Under these conditions, the body is integrated as a whole and all its parts relate with one another in movement (see http://www.caiwenyu.com. br/09_Fotos_p_ing.htm). These conditions cannot be produced by voluntary motor action, but emerge spontaneously with appropriate states of attention in which mechanosensing is enhanced. A person in such state could take advantage of elastic potential energy stored in the body when performing a blow. This characteristic of Qi Gong suggests a tensegrity-based mode of action with a high pre-stress level. In fact, potentiation of performance has already been shown in prestretched muscles, due to their ability to store potential elastic energy (Bosco et al., 2008; Ettema et al., 1990; Ishikawa et al., 2006).
Elements of martial arts training forms are often described in terms suggestive of animal pandiculatory patterns (Johnson, 2002). This invites reflection upon the fact that decorticated cats and dogs do exhibit instinctual behaviors, such as eating, copulating and fighting (Argyle, 1988); i.e., that basic life-supporting behaviors can happen without cortical participation. In fact, fighting appears to be a largely reflexive behavior, the expression of which is associated with subcortical structures such as the hypothalamus and midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Ulrich and Azrin, 1962; Shaikh and Siegel, 1994).
Figure 3: Dogs often pandiculate in this pattern of extension of the forelimbs and trunk; yawning is also frequently simultaneously present.
Pandiculation involves progressive cocontraction and stretching of soft tissues, which may extend to virtually every muscle in the body.
Figure 4: The downward dog yoga asana is reminiscent of a pandiculating dog.
Figure 5: Qi Gong posture. The stretching of the whole myofascial system is reminiscent of pandiculation.
Responses Induced by MR Might Be Similar to Those of Pandiculation
Like pandiculation, MR’s manual local loading of the myofascial system integrates body parts, apparently by inducing co-contraction of opposing muscle groups (Bertolucci, 2008; Bertolucci and Kozasa, 2010a, Bertolucci, 2010b), at the same time as it evokes a measurable rise in tonic muscle activity indicative of an overall increase in load. The client’s subjective experience is similar to that evoked by pandiculation, which suggests a common element among pandiculation, yoga and martial arts and MR.
In pandiculation, muscle activation begins locally and spreads to neighboring areas until it reaches a peak of distribution and intensity; i.e., joints progressively stiffen through a chain of reflexes, in which neighboring segments are sequentially engaged to form an ever-larger block that eventually encompasses the entire body. Following the peak, the tissues release.
MR induces a similar progressive engagement of body segments. The inclusion of each segment increases the overall tension within the block until, following the peak, the practitioner feels an abrupt soft tissue release. The progressive segmental engagement is paralleled by an increasing involuntary tonic muscle activity observable both by palpation and by electromyography (Bertolucci, 2008; Bertolucci and Kozasa, 2010a; Bertolucci, 2010b).
Let us imagine how it might be that MR and pandiculation would evoke similar muscle activity. The author hypothesizes that the manual forces applied during MR maneuvers mimic internal forces, and therefore induce mechanoreceptor afferents, similar to those characteristic of pandiculation.
In the clinical setting, recipients of MR treatments have exhibited spontaneous pandiculation-like movements (see videos at http://musclerepositioning. blogspot.com/), and have described their subjective experiences during MR as similar to their experiences during pandiculation. Some clients also report having resumed the habit of pandiculating in the morning, to which they attribute a greater sense of bodily well-being along with relief from musculoskeletal symptoms. These observations support the hypothesis of a similarity between MR and pandiculation.
Perhaps MR is a blend of myofascial release and “assisted pandiculation”, with the soft tissue release evoked by a combination of the practitioner’s manual input and the internally generated forces of tonic pandiculation-like reactions. This combination of forces might produce a greater effect in the soft tissues than either manual input or pandiculation alone.
Is SYS Culturally Inhibited?
SYS recruits the medullar, genetically determined Central Pattern Generators which, in higher primates, are under neocortical control (Marder and Rehm, 2005). Humans, unlike other primates, have been observed to yawn less frequently as they age (Walusinski et al., 2005); however, in various old world monkeys (Cercocebus albigena, Macaca fuscata and Macaca fascicularis) yawning has been shown to become more frequent as plasma androgen levels increase (Deputte, 1994; Troisi et al., 1990).
As yawning has negative social connotations in most cultures and religious traditions (Walusinski et al., 2010), perhaps cultural conditioning inhibits the SYS in humans. Given the likely homeostatic function of SYS, any such inhibition might contribute to the high incidence of human musculoskeletal disorders.
Biological Clocks and Health
Maintenance of homeostasis is closely related to biological rhythms, which are controlled by both environmental and endogenous stimuli (Aréchiga, 2003). Biological rhythms are generated by intracellular mechanisms called circadian clocks, which are present in both specific CNS centers and most peripheral tissues (Duguay and Cermakian, 2009; Aréchiga, 2003). In particular, they are present in fibroblasts, the cells responsible for the synthesis of ECM constituents. The intrinsic biological rhythms of fibroblasts have been shown to be involved in various metabolic functions (e.g., cytoskeleton protein synthesis, cell cycle control and enzyme synthesis, among others) (Bursian, 2009). While the fibroblast’s metabolic machinery has a rhythm of its own, it is at the same time influenced by mechanical stimuli—including, presumably, those generated through other biological rhythms. This raises the question of whether various biological rhythms mutually influence each other.
It is already known that various biological rhythms are related: e.g., heart rate, respiration, and periodic somatomotor excitation have been shown to interact (De Haro and Panda, 2006 Niizeki et al., 1993). The existence of such complex relationships among biological rhythms led to the concept of multi-oscillatory systems, as well as the hypothesis that synchronization among various co-existing rhythms is a determinant of gene expression and biological activity in both plants and animals (Lloyd, 2009; Bell- Pedersen et al., 2005). Quite possibly, the circadian clocks intrinsic to the fibroblasts might correlate regularly with other bio-cyclical phenomena such as the sleep-wake transition associated with SYS.
Sleep-wake and other biological rhythms have already been shown to be related to the immune response (Shepard and Shek, 1997), which indicates their importance to general health and inspires further study as to the possible role of rhythmic activities (such as SYS) in the maintenance of homeostasis in both particular tissues and in the organism as a whole. Tissue microdialysis, serum metabolic- tracers, neuroimaging, EEG and EMG are all possible resources for further investigation of the physiological role of SYS.
Conclusion
The concept of myofascial force transmission (Huijing and Jaspers, 2005) assumes the presence of ECM links among musculoskeletal components, which links unite those components into an integrated system; i.e., the fascia itself is assumed to play an integrative role. Integrated movement both requires and stimulates appropriate matrix connections. However, animals engage in a great deal of nonoptimal movement, of which immobilization (e.g., during sleep), trauma and bad postural habits are among the causes.
Should normal activities generate both “good” and “bad” mechanical signals to the ECM, the bad signals would need to be countermanded by good ones for the animal to maintain full movement capabilities throughout life. Pandiculation might provide one source of good signals by (i) breaking bad connections while stimulating better ones, and (ii) resetting postural muscle tonus to produce integrated movement, which movement is a further source of good mechanical signals. In short, pandiculation might be a form of neuro-myofascial hygiene.
If this be true, might we encourage pandiculation to enhance general health? This would require a reassessment of the cultural stigma against yawning and pandiculation, as well as further investigation of therapeutic approaches, such as Muscle Repositioning, that seem to stimulate it.
Acknowlegments
Thanks to Yeda Bocaletto, Angela Lobo, Soraia Pacchioni and Heidi Massa in the production of the manuscript.
References
Aréchiga, H., 2003. The neural substrate of biological rhythms. Revista de Neurologia 36 (1), 49-60.
Argiolas, A., Melis, M.R., 1998. The neuropharmacology of yawning. European Journal of Pharmacology 343 (1), 1-16. doi:10.1016/ S0014-2999(97)01538-0.
Argiolas, A., Melis, M.R., Murgia, S., Schiöth, H.B., 2000. ACTH- and alpha-MSH-induced grooming, stretching, yawning and penile erection in male rats: site of action in the brain and role of melanocortin receptors. Brain Research Bulletin 51 (5), 425-431.
Argyle, M., 1988. Bodily Communication. Routledge, New York.
Askenasy, J.J., 1989. Is yawing an arousal defense reflex? The Journal of Psychology 123 (6), 609-621.
Askenasy, J.J., Askenasy, N., 1996. Inhibition of muscle sympathetic nerve activity during yawning. Clinical Autonomic Research 6, 237-239. doi:10.1007/BF02291140.
Baenninger, R., 1997. On yawning and its functions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 4 (2), 198-207.
Banes, A.J., Horesovsky, G., Larson, C., Tsuzaki, M., Judex, S., Archambault, J., Zernicke, R., Herzog, W., Kelley, S., Miller, L., 1999. Mechanical load stimulates expression of novel genes in vivo and in vitro in avian flexor tendon cells. Osteoarthritis Cartilage 7 (1), 141-153. doi:10.1053/joca.1998.0169.
Bell-Pedersen, D., Cassone, V.M., Earnest, D.J., Golden, S.S., Hardin, P.E., Thomas, T.L., Zora, M.J., 2005. Circadian rhythms from multiple oscillators: lessons from diverse organisms. Nature Reviews Genetics 6 (7), 544e556. doi:10.1038/ nrg1633.
Bertolini, A., Gessa, G.L., 1981. Behavioral effects of ACTH and MSH peptides. Journal of Endocrinological Investigation 4, 241-251.
Bertolucci, L.F., 2008. Muscle Repositioning: “A new verifiable approach to neuro-myofascial release?”. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies 12, 213-224. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt. 2008.05.002.
Bertolucci, L.F., 2010b. Muscle Repositioning: combining subjective and objective feedbacks in the teaching and practice of a reflex-based myofascial release technique. International Journal of Therapeutic Massage and Bodywork 3 (1), 26-35.
Bertolucci, L.F., Kozasa, E.H., 2010a. Sustained manual loading of the fascial system can evoke tonic reactions: preliminary results. International Journal of Therapeutic Massage and Bodywork 3 (1), 12-14.
Bienfait, M., 1987. Bases physiologiques de la the´rapie manuelle. S.E.D. “Le Pousoe´”, Saint-Mont.
Bischofs, I.B., Klein, F., Lehnert, D., Bastmeyer, M., Schwarz, U.S., 2008. Filamentous network mechanics and active contractility determine cell and tissue shape. Biophysical Journal 95 (7), 3488e3496. doi:10.1529/biophysj.108.134296.
Bosco, C., Komi, P.V., Ito, A., 2008. Prestretch potentiation of human skeletal muscle during ballistic movement. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 111 (2), 135e140. doi:10.1111/j.1748- 1716. 1981.
Bouligand, Y., 1972. Twisted fibrous arrangement in biological materials and cholesteric mesophases. Tissue and Cell 4 (2), 189-217. doi:10.1016/S0040-8166(72)80042-9.
Bozarth, M.A., 1994. Pleasure systems in the brain. In: Warburton, D.M. (Ed.), Pleasure: The politics and the reality. John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp. 5-14.
Braunwald, E., Isselbacher, K.J., Petersdorf, R.G., Wilson, J.D., Martin, J.B., Fauci, A.S., 1987. Harrison’s Principles of Internal Medicine. McGraw-Hill, New York.
Briscoe, J., Wilkinson, D.G., 2004. Establishing neuronal circuitry: hox genes make the connection. Genes and Development 18 (14), 1643-1648. doi:10.1101/gad.1227004.
Burgdorf, J., Panksepp, J., 2006. The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews 30 (2), 173-187. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.001.
Bursian, A.V., 2009. Organization of endogenous rhythms of motor functions. Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology 45 (6), 663-669. doi:10.1134/S0022093009060039.
Cabanac, M., 1992. Pleasure: the common currency. Journal of Theoretical Biology 155 (2), 173-200.
Chen, C.S., Ingber, D.E., 1999. Tensegrity and mechanoregulation: from skeleton to cytoskeleton. Osteoarthritis and Cartilage 7 (1), 81e94. doi:10.1053/joca.1998.0164.
Chernev, I., Petrea, R.E., Reynolds, M.S., Wang, F., 2009. The classical type of Foix-Chavany-Marie syndrome: assessment and treatment of dysphagia. The Internet Journal of Neurology 11 (1).
Chiquet, M., Gelman, L., Lutz, R., Maier, S., 2009. From mechanotransduction to extracellular matrix gene expression in fibroblasts. Biochimica and Biophysica Acta 1793 (5), 911-920. doi:10.1016/j.bbamcr.2009.01.012.
Craig, A.D., 2003. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13 (4), 500-505. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4.
Dahl, K.N., Booth-Gauthier, E.A., Ladoux, B., 2010. In the middle of it all: mutual mechanical regulation between the nucleus and the cytoskeleton. Journal of Biomechanics 43 (1), 2-8. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2009.09.002.
De Haro, L., Panda, S., 2006. Systems biology of circadian rhythms: an outlook. Journal of Biological Rhythms 21 (6), 507-518. doi: 10.1177/0748730406294767.
de Vries, J.I., Visser, G.H., Prechtl, H.F., 1982. The emergence of fetal behavior: I. Qualitative Aspects Early Human Development 7 (4), 301-322. doi:10.1016/0378-3782(82)90033-0.
De Wied, D., 1999. Behavioral pharmacology of neuropeptides related to melanocortins and the neurohypophyseal hormones. European Journal of Pharmacology 375 (1-3), 1e11. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(99)00339-8.
Dentona, D.A., McKinleyc, M.J., Farrell, M., Egan, G.F., 2009. The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 18 (2), 500-514. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.009.
Deputte, B.L., 1994. Ethological study of yawning in primates. I: Quantitative analysis and study of causation in two species of old world monkeys (Cercocebus albigena and Macaca fascicularis). Ethology 98 (3-4), 221e245.
Döbereiner, H.G., Dubin-Thaler, B.J., Giannone, G., Sheetz, M.P., 2005. Force sensing and generation in cell phases: analyses of complex functions. Journal of Applied Physiology 98, 1542-1546. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.01181.2004.
Duguay, D., Cermakian, N., 2009. The crosstalk between physiology and circadian clock proteins. Chronobiology International 26 (8), 1479-1513.
Esch, T., Stefano, G.B., 2004. The neurobiology of pleasure, reward processes, addiction and their health implications. Neuroendocrinology Letters 4 (25), 235-251.
Ettema, G.J.C., Van Soest, A.J., Huijing, P.A., 1990. The role of series elastic structures in prestretch-induced work enhancement during isotonic and isokinetic contractions. Journal of Experimental Biology 154, 121-136.
Fraser, A.F., 1989a. Pandiculation: the comparative phenomenon of systematic stretching. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 23, 263-268. doi:10.1016/0168-1591(89)90117-2.
Fraser, A.F., 1989b. The phenomenon of pandiculation in the kinetic behaviour of the sheep fetus. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 24 (2), 169-182. doi:10.1016/0168-1591(89)90044-0.
Gieni, R.S., Hendzel, M.J., 2008. Mechanotransduction from the ECM to the genome: are the pieces now in place? Journal of Cellular Biochemistry 104 (6), 1964-1987. doi:10.1002/jcb.21364.
Giraud-Guille, M.M., 1992. Liquid crystallinity in condensed type I collagen solutions. A clue to the packing of collagen in extracellular matrices. Journal of Molecular Biology 224 (3), 861-873.
Giraud-Guille, M.M., Besseau, L., Martin, R., 2003. Liquid crystalline assemblies of collagen in bone and in vitro systems. Journal of Biomechanics 36 (10), 1571-1579. doi:10.1016/S0021- 9290(03)00134-9.
Graham, M., 1982. Assoctiated reactions in the hemiplegic arm. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 14 (3), 117-120.
Guyton, A.C., Hall, J.E., 2006. Textbook of Medical Physiology. Elsevier, Philadelphia.
Hallett, M., 2007. Volitional control of movement: the physiology of free will. Clinical Neurophysiology 118 (6), 1179-1192. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.03.019.
Hanna, T., 2004. Somatics: Reawakening the Mind’s Control of Movement, Flexibility, and Health. Da Capo Press, Cambridge.
Haudenschild, A.K., Hsieh, A.H., Kapila, S., Lotz, J.C., 2009. Pressure and distortion regulate human mesenchymal stem cell gene expression. Annals Biomedical Engeneering 37 (3), 492-502. doi:10.1007/s10439-008-9629-2.
Heinemeier, K.M., Olesen, J.L., Haddad, F., Langberg, H., Kjaer, M., Baldwin, K.M., Schjerling, P., 2007. Expression of collagen and related growth factors in rat tendon and skeletal muscle in response to specific contraction types. The Journal of Physiology 582 (3), 1303-1316. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2007.127639.
Heusner, A.P., 1946. Yawning and associated phenomena. Physiological Reviews 26, 156-168. Holstege, G., 1992. The emotional motor system. European Journal of Morphology 30 (1), 67-79.
Hommel, B., 2009. Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research 73 (4), 512-526. doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0234-2.
Hopf, H.C., M?ller-Forell, W., Hopf, N.J., 1992. Localization of emotional and volitional facial paresis. Neurology 42 (10), 1918-1923.
Huijing, P.A., Jaspers, R.T., 2005. Adaptation of muscle size and myofascial force transmission: a review and some new experimental results. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports 15 (6), 349e380. doi:10.1111/j.1600- 0838.2005.00457.x.
Humphrey, J.D., Delange, S.L., 2004. An Introduction to Biomechanics: Solids and Fluids, Analysis and Design. Springer, NewYork.
Hwang, I.S., Tung, L.C., Yang, J.F., Chen, Y.C., Yeh, C.Y., Wang, C.H., 2005. Electromyographic analyses of global synkinesis in the paretic upper limb after stroke. Physical Therapy 85 (8), 755e765.
Ingber, D.E., 1998. The architecture of life. Scientific American 278 (1), 48-57.
Ingber, D.E., 2006. Cellular mechanotransduction: putting all the pieces together again. FASEBFederation of American Societies for Experimental Biology Journal 20 (7), 811-827.
Ingber, D.E., 2008a. Tensegrity and mechanotransduction. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies 12 (3), 198-200. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2008.04.038.
Ingber, D.E., 2008b. Tensegrity-based mechanosensing from macro to micro. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology 97 (2-3), 163-179. doi:10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2008.02.005.
Ishikawa, M., Komi, P.V., Finni, T., Kuitunen, S., 2006. Contribution of the tendinous tissue to force enhancement during stretcheshortening cycle exercise depends on the prestretch and concentric phase intensities. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology 16 (5), 423-431.
Iyengar, B.K.S., 1979. Light on Yoga. Schocken Books, New York.
Jacobs, J.V., Horak, F.B., 2007. Cortical control of postural responses. Journal of Neural Transmission 114 (10), 1339-1348. doi:10.1007/s00702-007-0657-0.
Johnson, R., 2002. Boa forma para preguiçosos: Joyflexing. Editora Pensamento-Cultrix, São Paulo.
Jones, D.A., Round, J.,Haan, A., 2004. Skeletal Muscle from Molecules to Movement. A Textbook of Muscle Physiology for Sport, Exercise, Physiotherapy and Medicine. Churchill Livingstone, London.
Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., Jessell, T.M., 2000. Principles of Neural Science. McGraw-Hill, New York. Kapandji, I.A., 1987. Fisiologia articular: Esquemas comentados de mecânica humana. Ed Manole, São Paulo.
Kjær, M., 2004. Role of extracellular matrix in adaptation of tendon and skeletal muscle to mechanical loading. Physiological Reviews 84 (2), 649-698. doi:10.1152/physrev.00031.2003.
Kjær, M., Langberg, H., Heinemeier, K., Bayer, M.L., Hansen, M., Holm, L., Doessing, S., Kongsgaard, M., Krogsgaard, M.R., Magnusson, S.P., 2009. From mechanical loading to collagen synthesis, structural changes and function in human tendon. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports 19 (4), 500-510.
Kjaer, M., Langberg, H., Miller, B.F., Boushel, R., Crameri, R., Koskinen, S., Heinemeier, K., Olesen, J.L., Døssing, S., Hansen, M., Pedersen, S.G., Rennie, M.J., Magnusson, P., 2005. Metabolic activity and collagen turnover in human tendon in response to physical activity. Journal of Musculoskeletal and Neuronal Interactions 5 (1), 41-52.
Kjær, M., Magnusson, P., Krogsgaard, M., Boysen Møller, J., Olesen, J., Heinemeier, K., Hansen, M., Haraldsson, B., Koskinen, S., Esmarck, B., Langberg, H., 2006. Extracellular matrix adaptation of tendon and skeletal muscle to exercise. Journal of Anatomy 208 (4), 445-450.
Kreis, R., Boesch, C., 1994. Liquid-crystal-like structure of human muscle demonstrated by in vivo observation of direct dipolar coupling in localized proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Journal of Magnetic Resonance B 10 (2), 189-192. 4.
Lagercrantz, H., Ringstedt, T., 2001. Organization of the neuronal circuits in the central nervous system during development. Acta Paediatrica 90 (7), 707-715.
Lappa, M., 2003. The growth and the fluid dynamics of protein crystals and soft organic tissues: models and simulations, similarities and differences. Journal of Theoretical Biology 224 (2), 225-240.
Lehmann, H.E., 1979. Yawning: a homeostatic reflex and its psychological significance. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 43 (2), 123-136.
Leikin, S., Rau, D.C., Parsegian, V.A., 1995. Temperature-favoured assembly of collagen is driven by hydrophilic not hydrophobic interactions. Nature Structural Biology 2 (3), 205-210. doi: 10.1038/nsb0395-205.
Lloyd, D., 2009. Oscillations, synchrony and deterministic chaos. Progress in Botany 70, 69-91. doi:10.1007/978-3-540- 68421-3_4.
Magoun, H.W., Ranson, S.W., 1938. The behavior of cats following Bilateral Removal of the Rostral Portion of the cerebral Hemispheres. Journal of Neurophysiology 1, 39-44.
Marder, E., Rehm, K.J., 2005. Development of central pattern generating circuits. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15 (1), 86e93. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.01.011.
Moore, S.W., 2003. Scrambled eggs: mechanical forces as ecological factors in early development. Evolution and Development 5 (1), 61-66. doi:10.1046/j.1525-142X.2003.03010.x.
Muni, S.W., 1994. Awakening the life force: The philosophy and psychology of “spontaneous yoga”. Llewellyn Publications, Minnesota.
Niizeki, K., Kawahara, K., Miyamoto, Y., 1993. Interaction among cardiac, respiratory, and locomotor rhythms during cardiolocomotor synchronization. Journal of Applied Physiology 75 (4), 1815-1821.
Oman, R.E., Sullivan, S.J., Fleury, J., Dutil, E., 1989. Yawning: a possible Confounding Variable in EMG Biofeedback studies Biofeedback and self-Regulation. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 14 (4), 339-346. doi:10.1007/ BF00999125.
Patwari, P., Lee, R.T., 2008. Mechanical control of tissue morphogenesis. Circulation Research 103 (3), 234-243. doi: 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.108.175331.
Pincus, J.H., Tucker, G.J., 1974. Behavioral Neurology. Oxford University Press, New York.
Recordati, G., Bellini, T.G., 2004. A definition of internal constancy and homeostasis in the context of non-equilibrium thermodynamics. Experimental Physiology 89 (1), 27-38. doi:10.1113/ expphysiol.2003.002633.
Rieffel, J.A., Valero-Cuevas, F.J., Lipson, H., 2010. Morphological communication: exploiting coupled dynamics in a complex mechanical structure to achieve locomotion. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 7, 613-621. doi:10.1098/ rsif.2009.0240.
Rosenbloom, L., 1994. Motor development in early and later childhood: longitudinal approaches. Archieves of Disease Childhood 71 (4), 391.
Russell, J., Fernández-Dols, J.M., 1997. The Psychology of Facial Expression. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Sadler, T.W., 1995. Langman’s Medical Embriology, seventh ed.. Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore.
Sandyk, R., 1998. Yawning and stretching-a behavioral syndrome associated with transcranial application of electromagnetic fields in multiple sclerosis. International Journal of Neuroscience 95 (1-2), 107-113. doi:10.3109/00207459809000654.
Sandyk, R., 1999. Yawning and stretching induced by transcranial application of AC pulsed electromagnetic fields in Parkinson’s disease. International Journal of Neuroscience 97 (1-2), 139-145. doi:10.3109/00207459908994308.
Sato-Suzuki, I., Kita, I., Oguri, M., Arita, H., 1998. Stereotyped yawning responses induced by electrical and chemical stimulation of paraventricular nucleus of the rat. Journal of Neurophysiology 80 (5), 2765-2775.
Sato-Suzuki, I., Kita, I., Seki, Y., Oguri, M., Arita, H., 2002. Cortical arousal induced by microinjection of orexins into the paraventricular nucleus of the rat. Behavioral Brain Research 128 (2), 169-177. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00307-2.
Sauer, E.G., Sauer, E.M., 1967. Yawning and other maintenance activities in the South African Ostrich. The Auk 84, 571-587.
Shaikh, M.B., Siegel, A., 1994. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical mechanisms underlying amygdaloid control of defensive rage behavior in the cat. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research 27 (12), 2759-2779. Dec.
Shephard, R.J., Shek, P.N., 1997. Interactions between sleep, other body rhythms, immune responses, and exercise. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology 22 (2), 95-116.
Sherwood, L., 2010. Human Physiology: From Cells to Systems, seventh ed.. Brooks/Cole, Belmont CA.
Silver, F.H., Siperko, L.M., Seehra, G.P., 2003. Mechanobiology of force transduction in dermal tissue. Skin Research and Technology 9 (1), 3e23. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0846.2003.00358.x.
Smith, K.U., 1939. The behavior of decorticate guinea pigs. Journal of Comparative Psychology 27 (3), 433-447.
Smith, R., 1992. Inhibition: History and Meaning in the Sciences of Mind and Brain. University of California Press, Berkeley.
Stevens-Tuttle, D., Fox, J., Bouffard, N.A., Henry, S., Wu, J., Langevin, H.M., 2008. Perimuscular fascia remodeling in a Porcine movement restriction model relevant to human low back pain. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies 13 (1), 91. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt.2008.04.011.
Stewart, J.P., 1921. On muscle-tonus, tonic rigidity, and tonic Fits. British Medical Journal 1 (3137), 217-219.
Streuli, C.H., 2009. Integrins and cell-fate determination. Journal of Cell Science 122 (2), 171-177. doi:10.1242/10.1242/ jcs.018945.
Takakusaki, K., Ohinata-Sugimoto, J., Saitoh, K., Habaguchi, T., 2003. Role of basal ganglia-brainstem systems in the control of postural muscle tone and locomotion. Progresss in Brain Research 143, 231-237.
Tomiosso, T.C., Gomes, L., de Campos Vidal, B., Pimentel, E.R., 2005. Extracellular matrix of ostrich articular cartilage. Biocell 29 (1), 47-54.
Töpper, R., Kosinski, C., Mull, M., 1995. Volitional type of facial palsy associated with pontine ischaemia. Journal of Neurology. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 58 (6), 732-734. doi:10.1136/ jnnp.58.6.732.
Töpper, R., Mull, M., Nacimiento, W., 2003. Involuntary stretching during yawning in patients with pyramidal tract lesions: further evidence for the existence of an independent emotional motor system. European Journal of Neurology 10 (5), 495-499. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2003.00599.x.
Troisi, A., Aureli, F., Schino, G., Rinaldi, F., De Angeli, N., 1990. The influence of age, sex, rank on yawning behavior in two species of macaques (Macaca fascicularis, Macaca fuscata). Ethology 86, 303e310.
Ulrich, R.E., Azrin, N.H., 1962. Reflexive fighting in response to aversive stimulation1 Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior. October 5 (4), 511-520. doi:10.1901/jeab.1962.5-511.
Urba-Holmgren, R., Gonzalez, R.M., Holmgren, B., 1977. Is yawning cholinergic response? Nature 267 (5608), 261-262. doi: 10.1038/267261a0.
Vergoni, A.V., Bertolini, A., Mutulis, F., Wikberg, J.E., Schiöth, H.B., 1998. Differential influence of a selective melanocortin MC4 receptor antagonist (HS014) on melanocortin-induced behavioral effects in rats. European Journal of Pharmacology 362 (2-3), 95e101. doi:10.1038/267261a0.
Vincent, J.D., 1994. Biology of pleasure. Presse Me´dicale 23 (40), 1871-1876.
Vishnivetz, B., 1995. Educaçao do corpo para o ser. Editora Summus, São Paulo. Walshe, F.M.R., 1923. On certain tonic or postural reflexes in hemiplegia with special reference to the so called “associated movements. Brain 46, 1-37.
Walusinski, O., 2006. Yawning: unsuspected avenue for a better understanding of arousal and interoception. Medical Hypotheses 67 (1), 6-14. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2006.01.020.
Walusinski, O., Kurjak, A., Andonotopo, W., Azumendi, G., 2005. Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography. The Ultrasound Review of Obstetrics and Gynecology 5 (3), 210-217. doi: 10.1080/14722240500284070.
Walusinski, O., Meenakshisundaram, R., Thirumalaikolundusubramanian, P., Diwakar, S., Dhanalakshmi, G., 2010. Yawning: Comparative Study of Knowledge and Beliefs, Popular and Medica. In the Mystery of Yawning in Physiology and Disease. Available at. http://www.baillement.com/recherche/beliefs_ knowledge.pdf.
Wolff, J., 1986. The Law of Bone Remodelling. Springer-Verlag, Berlim.Hypothesis—Pandiculation[:]
As you register, you allow [email protected] to send you emails with information
The language of this site is in English, but you can navigate through the pages using the Google Translate. Just select the flag of the language you want to browse. Automatic translation may contain errors, so if you prefer, go back to the original language, English.
Developed with by Empreiteira Digital
To have full access to the content of this article you need to be registered on the site. Sign up or Register.